Simple Behavioral Assessment of Mouse Olfaction

Mu Yang1, Jacqueline N. Crawley1

1 National Institute of Mental Health, NIH, Bethesda, Maryland
Publication Name:  Current Protocols in Neuroscience
Unit Number:  Unit 8.24
DOI:  10.1002/0471142301.ns0824s48
Online Posting Date:  July, 2009
GO TO THE FULL TEXT: PDF or HTML at Wiley Online Library

Abstract

This unit presents two basic protocols that offer rapid assessments of anosmia (the absence of a sense of smell) in mice. The buried food test is used to check for the ability to smell volatile odors. The olfactory habituation/dishabituation test is used to test whether the animal can detect and differentiate different odors, including both nonsocial and social odors. A non‐contact method of odor presentation, along with a general method for collecting urine samples, is given as an alternate protocol. The tests described in this unit only require simple equipment and can be adopted readily by most laboratories.Curr. Protoc. Neurosci. 48:8.24.1‐8.24.12. © 2009 by John Wiley & Sons, Inc.

Keywords: olfaction; mouse; anosmia; buried food test; habituation/dishabituation test

     
 
GO TO THE FULL PROTOCOL:
PDF or HTML at Wiley Online Library

Table of Contents

  • Introduction
  • Basic Protocol 1: Buried Food Test
  • Basic Protocol 2: The Olfactory Habituation/Dishabituation Test
  • Alternate Protocol 1: Testing Olfactory Habituation/Dishabituation with Noncontactable Odor Stimuli
  • Support Protocol 1: Urine Collection from Mice
  • Commentary
  • Literature Cited
  • Figures
     
 
GO TO THE FULL PROTOCOL:
PDF or HTML at Wiley Online Library

Materials

Basic Protocol 1: Buried Food Test

  Materials
  • Mice: at least 6 weeks of age (juveniles may respond differently to food deprivation)
  • Food stimulus (particles should each weigh ∼1.5 g): Teddy Grahams (Nabisco) or other mini‐cookies of uniform size; other choices include sweetened cereal such as Froot Loops (Kellogg's), chocolate chips, squares of hard cheese, or small pellets of standard chow
  • Clean mouse cages (standard plastic cages, 46 cm L × 23.5 cm W × 20 cm H, or similar); the stainless steel top which holds food and water interferes with observation and should not be included in the setup.
  • Fresh cage bedding to create a 3‐cm layer in each cage
  • Clean holding cage for each subject for holding subject temporarily while stimulus is being buried; holding cages should be placed close to the testing cage
  • Digital timer: to monitor the test length (maximum = 15 min)
  • Traceable 300‐Memory Stopwatch (Control Company; http://www.control3.com)

Basic Protocol 2: The Olfactory Habituation/Dishabituation Test

  Materials
  • Mice: 6 weeks of age or older (sexually mature)
  • Odor stimuli:
    • Non‐social odors (use distilled H 2O as solvent): (1) distilled H 2O, (2) almond extract (McCormick; 1:100 dilution), (3) imitation banana extract (McCormick; 1:100 dilution), or similar; prepare fresh on day of experiment (almond and banana extract are selected because they are distinct yet neutral odors; to ensure consistent odor potency, replace concentrated extract every few months; store extracts away from direct sunlight and heat)
    • Social odors are obtained from mice of the same or opposite sex as the subject, depending on the scientific question (see below)
  • Clean mouse cages (standard plastic cages, 46 cm L × 23.5 cm W × 20 cm H, or similar), containing only fresh wood chip bedding
  • Clean plastic cage lids, one for each cage
  • Cotton‐tipped wooden applicators (6 in. length; Solon Manufacturing Company, http://www.solonme.com/), or similar
  • Small (4.5 × 4.5 cm) weigh boat
  • Digital stopwatches (silenced, as described in protocol 1) to record sniffing time
  • Digital timer to register the 2‐min trial duration
  • Glass vials for water and solutions (label clearly and keep tightly capped when not in use)
  • Large lidded jar for disposal of used applicators (a temporary disposal container helps to prevent cross‐trial contamination; empty jar after testing each animal)

Alternate Protocol 1: Testing Olfactory Habituation/Dishabituation with Noncontactable Odor Stimuli

  Materials
  • Mice: at least 6 weeks of age (sexually mature)
  • Odorant (e.g., urine, see protocol 4)
  • Fine wire mesh
  • Clean mouse cages (standard plastic cages, 46 cm L × 23.5 cm W × 20 cm H, or similar), containing only fresh wood chip bedding
  • Cage lids, including the plastic top and the stainless steel grid top
  • Small plastic weigh boats (4.5 cm × 4.5 cm)
  • Filter paper, cut into individual squares (2 cm × 2 cm)
  • Double‐sided tape
  • Digital stopwatches (silenced, as described in protocol 1)
  • Digital timer to register trial duration
  • Large lidded jar to dispose of pipet tips and pieces of filter paper

Support Protocol 1: Urine Collection from Mice

  Materials
  • Adult mice, 6 weeks of age or older
  • Microcentrifuge tubes or clean glass vials
  • Whatman no. 5 filter paper
  • Small sealable plastic bags
GO TO THE FULL PROTOCOL:
PDF or HTML at Wiley Online Library

Figures

Videos

Literature Cited

Literature Cited
   Achiraman, S. and Archunan, G. 2006. 1‐Iodo‐2methylundecane, a putative estrus‐specific urinary chemo‐signal of female mouse (Mus musculus). Theriogenology 66:1913‐1920.
   Alberts, J.R. and Galef, B.G. Jr. 1971. Acute anosmia in the rat: A behavioral test of a peripherally‐induced olfactory deficit. Physiol. Behav. 6:619‐621.
   Bainbridge, N.K., Koselke, L.R., Jeon, J., Bailey, K.R., Wess, J., Crawley, J.N., and Wrenn, C.C. 2008. Learning and memory impairments in a congenic C57BL/6 strain of mice that lacks the M2 muscarinic acetylcholine receptor subtype. Behav. Brain Res. 190:50‐58.
   Bakker, J., Honda, S., Harada, N., and Balthazart, J. 2002. The aromatase knock‐out mouse provides new evidence that estradiol is required during development in the female for the expression of sociosexual behaviors in adulthood. J. Neurosci. 22:9104‐9112.
   Brennan, P.A. and Keverne, E.B. 2004. Something in the air? New insights into mammalian pheromones. Curr. Biol. 14:R81‐R89.
   Brennan, P.A. and Zufall, F. 2006. Pheromonal communication in vertebrates. Nature 444:308‐315.
   Brown, R.E. 1979. Mammalian social odors. Adv. Stud. Behav. 10:107‐161.
   Crawley, J.N., Chen, T., Puri, A., Washburn, R., Sullivan, T.L., Hill, J.M., Young, N.B., Nadler, J.J., Moy, S.S., Young, L.J., Caldwell, H.K., and Young, W.S. 2007. Social approach behaviors in oxytocin knockout mice: Comparison of two independent lines tested in different laboratory environments. Neuropeptides 41:145‐163.
   Dawson, P.A., Steane, S.E., and Markovich, D. 2005. Impaired memory and olfactory performance in NaSi‐1 sulphate transporter deficient mice. Behav. Brain Res. 159:15‐20.
   Del Punta, K., Leinders‐Zufall, T., Rodriguez, I., Jukam, D., Wysocki, C.J., Ogawa, S., Zufall, F., and Mombaerts, P., 2002. Deficient pheromone responses in mice lacking a cluster of vomeronasal receptor genes. Nature 419:70‐74.
   Doty, R.L. 1986. Odor‐guided behavior in mammals. Experientia 42:257‐271.
   Edward, D.A., Thompson, M.L., and Burge, K.G. 1972. Olfactory bulb removal vs. peripherally induced anosmia: Differential effects on the aggression behavior of male mice. Behav. Biol. 7:823‐828.
   Gregg, B. and Thiessen, D.D. 1981. A simple method of olfactory discrimination of urines for the Mongolian gerbil, Meriones unguiculatus. Physiol Behav. 26:1133‐1136.
   Jakupovic, J., Kang, N., and Baum, M.J. 2008. Effect of bilateral accessory olfactory bulb lesions on volatile urinary odor discrimination and investigation as well as mating behavior in male mice. Physiol. Behav. 93:467‐473.
   Jamain, S., Radyushkin, K., Hammerschmidt, K., Granon, S., Boretius, S., Varoqueaux, F., Ramanantsoa, N., Gallego, J., Ronnenberg, A., Winter, D., Frahm, J., Fischer, J., Bourgeron, T., Ehrenreich, H., and Brose, N. 2008. Reduced social interaction and ultrasonic communication in a mouse model of monogenic heritable autism. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105:1710‐1715.
   Kavaliers, M., Choleris, E., Agmo, A., Braun, W.J., Colwell, D.D., Muglia, L.J., Ogawa, S., and Pfaff, D.W. 2006. Inadvertent social information and the avoidance of parasitized male mice: A role for oxytocin. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 103:4293‐4298.
   Keller, M., Douhard, Q., Baum, M.J., and Bakker, J. 2006. Sexual experience does not compensate for the disruptive effects of zinc sulfate lesioning of the main olfactory epithelium on sexual behavior in male mice. Chem. Senses 31:753‐762.
   Kern, R.C., Conley, D.B., Haines, G.K. 3rd, and Robinson, A.M. 2004. Treatment of olfactory dysfunction, II: Studies with minocycline. Laryngoscope 114:2200‐2204.
   Keverne, E.B. 2004. Importance of olfactory and vomeronasal systems for male sexual function. Physiol. Behav. 83:177‐187.
   Klein, S.L., Kriegsfeld, L.J., Hairston, J.E., Rau, V., Nelson, R.J., and Yarowsky, P.J. 1996. Characterization of sensorimotor performance, reproductive and aggressive behaviors in segmental trisomic 16 (Ts65Dn) mice. Physiol. Behav. 60:1159‐1164.
   Luo, A.H., Cannon, E.H., Wekesa, K.S., Lyman, R.F., Vandenbergh, J.G., and Anholt, R.R. 2002. Impaired olfactory behavior in mice deficient in the alpha subunit of G(o). Brain Res. 941:62‐71.
   Macknin, J.B., Higuchi, M., Lee, V.M., Trojanowski, J.Q., and Doty, R.L. 2004. Olfactory dysfunction occurs in transgenic mice overexpressing human tau protein. Brain Res. 1000:174‐178.
   Martel, K.L. and Baum, M.J. 2007. Sexually dimorphic activation of the accessory, but not the main, olfactory bulb in mice by urinary volatiles. Eur. J. Neurosci. 26:463‐475.
   Martel, K.L., Keller, M., Douhard, Q., Bakker, J., and Baum, M.J. 2007. Comparison of urinary odor‐induced glomerular activation in the main olfactory bulb of aromatase knock‐out and wild type female mice. Neurosci Lett. 421:101‐105.
   Quiroz‐Padilla, M.F., Guillazo‐Blanch, G., Vale‐Martinez, A., and Marti‐Nicolovius, M. 2006. Excitotoxic lesions of the parafascicular nucleus produce deficits in a socially transmitted food preference. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 86:256‐263.
   Restrepo, D., Arellano, J., Oliva, A.M., Schaefer, M.L., and Lin, W. 2004. Emerging views on the distinct but related roles of the main and accessory olfactory systems in responsiveness to chemosensory signals in mice. Horm. Behav. 46:247‐256.
   Sanchez‐Andrade, G., James, B.M., and Kendrick, K.M. 2005. Neural encoding of olfactory recognition memory. J. Reprod. Develop. 51:547‐558.
   Schellinck, H.M., Smyth, C., Brown, R., and Wilkinson, M. 1993. Odor‐induced sexual maturation and expression of c‐fos in the olfactory system of juvenile female mice. Brain Res. Dev. Brain Res. 74:138‐141.
   Stack, C.M., Lim, M.A., Cuasay, K., Stone, M.M., Seibert, K.M., Spivak‐Pohis, I., Crawley, J.N., Waschek, J.A., and Hill, J.M. 2008. Deficits in social behavior and reversal learning are more prevalent in male offspring of VIP deficient female mice. Exp. Neurol. 211:67‐84.
   Trinh, K. and Storm, D.R. 2003. Vomeronasal organ detects odorants in absence of signaling through main olfactory epithelium. Nat. Neurosci. 6:519‐525.
   Wersinger, S.R. and Rissman, E.F. 2000. Oestrogen receptor alpha is essential for female‐directed chemo‐investigatory behaviour but is not required for the pheromone‐induced luteinizing hormone surge in male mice. J. Neuroendocrinol. 12:103‐110.
   Wersinger, S.R., Kelliher, K.R., Zufall, F., Lolait, S.J., O'Carroll, A.M., and Young, W.S. 3rd. 2004. Social motivation is reduced in vasopressin 1b receptor null mice despite normal performance in an olfactory discrimination task. Horm. Behav. 46:638‐645.
   Wersinger, S.R., Caldwell, H.K., Martinez, L., Gold, P., Hu, S.B., and Young, W.S. 3rd. 2007. Vasopressin 1a receptor knockout mice have a subtle olfactory deficit but normal aggression. Genes Brain Behav. 6:540‐551.
   Wesson, D.W., Keller, M., Douhard, Q., Baum, M.J., and Bakker, J. 2006. Enhanced urinary odor discrimination in female aromatase knockout (ArKO) mice. Horm. Behav. 49:580‐586.
   Wesson, D.W., Donahou, T.N., Johnson, M.O., and Wachowiak, M. 2008. Sniffing behavior of mice during performance in odor‐guided tasks. Chem. Senses 33:581‐596.
   Wong, S.T., Trinh, K., Hacker, B., Chan, G.C., Lowe, G., Gaggar, A., Xia, Z., Gold, G.H., and Storm, D.R. 2000. Disruption of the type III adenylyl cyclase gene leads to peripheral and behavioral anosmia in transgenic mice. Neuron 27:487‐497.
   Woodley, S.K. and Baum, M.J. 2003. Effects of sex hormones and gender on attraction thresholds for volatile anal scent gland odors in ferrets. Horm. Behav. 44:110‐118.
   Wrenn, C.C., Kinney, J.W., Marriott, L.K., Holmes, A., Harris, A.P., Saavedra, M.C., Starosta, G., Innerfield, C.E., Jacoby, A.S., Shine, J., Iismaa, T.P., Wenk, G.L., and Crawley, J.N. 2004. Learning and memory performance in mice lacking the GAL‐R1 subtype of galanin receptor. Eur. J. Neurosci. 19:1384‐1396.
   Yamada, K., Wada, E., and Wada, K. 2001. Female gastrin‐releasing peptide receptor (GRP‐R)‐deficient mice exhibit altered social preference for male conspecifics: Implications for GRP/GRP‐R modulation of GABAergic function. Brain Res. 894:281‐287.
   Young, W.S. 3rd, Shepard, E., Amico, J., Hennighausen, L., Wagner, K.U., LaMarca, M.E., McKinney, C., and Ginns, E.I. 1996. Deficiency in mouse oxytocin prevents milk ejection, but not fertility or parturition. J. Neuroendocrinol. 8:847‐853.
GO TO THE FULL PROTOCOL:
PDF or HTML at Wiley Online Library